Elsevier

Schizophrenia Research

Volume 81, Issues 2–3, 31 January 2006, Pages 261-267
Schizophrenia Research

Reduced perception of the motion-induced blindness illusion in schizophrenia

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2005.08.012Get rights and content

Abstract

Motion-induced blindness (MIB) occurs when target stimuli are presented together with a moving distractor pattern. Most observers experience the targets disappearing and reappearing repeatedly for periods of up to several seconds. MIB can be viewed as a striking marker for the organization of cognitive functioning. In the present study, MIB rates and durations were assessed in 34 schizophrenia-spectrum disorder patients and matched controls. The results showed that positive symptoms and excitement enhanced MIB, whereas depression and negative symptoms attenuated the illusion. MIB was more frequently found in normal subjects. The results remained consistent after adjusting for reaction time and error rates. Hence, MIB may provide a valid and reliable measure of cognitive organization in schizophrenia.

Introduction

Research on cognitive organization in schizophrenia dates back to the gestalt school of psychiatry (Matussek, 1952). The general hypothesis according to which symptoms of schizophrenia arise from the dysfunctional processing of visual patterns has found support in a number of investigations into diverse tasks requiring perceptual organization (Uhlhaas and Silverstein, 2003, Uhlhaas and Silverstein, in press, Silverstein et al., 2000, Rabinowicz et al., 1996). Moreover, several reports indicate correlations between perceptual organization and psychopathology, instances of which include schizotypy, the disorganization syndrome, as well as positive and negative symptoms (Goodarzi et al., 2000, Doniger et al., 2001, Parnas et al., 2001).

Perception coordinates sensory input in a manner that commonly goes unnoticed in natural settings, but can be strikingly demonstrated in a laboratory setting. A well-known example is figure/ground reversal in ambiguous figures. In Fig. 1 (Rubin, 1921), the viewer either perceives two black silhouettes or a white vase, but never both at the same time. Several illusions have been described in which specific areas or aspects of a stimulus field are completely disregarded. For instance, the phenomenon of change blindness or inattentional blindness is the inability of viewers to spot even highly conspicuous changes in areas of the scene not focused on (Milner and Goodale, 1995). A remarkable demonstration of inattentional blindness was reported by Simons and Chabris (1999), who instructed subjects watching a ball game to count the number of passes made by the team wearing white jerseys. It was found that clearly visible events (in this trial, a gorilla walking across the middle of the playing field) may go completely unnoticed. Perceptual filling-in (Ramachandran and Gregory, 1991) occurs when an homogeneous gray square lying peripheral to the center of vision is displayed against a background of twinkling two-dimensional noise made up of black and white dots. On steady eccentric fixation, the square vanishes and is filled in by the surrounding twinkling noise.

The present study focused on the similar phenomenon of motion-induced blindness (MIB) as recently described by Bonneh et al. (2001) in a small sample of normal observers. In this trial, yellow stationary dots were presented as target stimuli on a screen, while a distractor pattern–in this case, a cloud of low-contrast blue dots–rotated next to the targets. A large majority of the sample perceived the salient target dots vanishing and reappearing repeatedly for periods of several seconds at a time. The phenomenon could be reproduced reliably over a range of pattern variations. The effect depended, however, on the movement of the distractor pattern. Bonneh et al. suggested that this illusion probably reflects attentional mechanisms operating in a “winner-takes-all strategy.” Presumably, MIB cannot be explained by low hierarchy processes, such as retinal suppression or contrast maximizing. This supposition has since been corroborated by a study on the intensity of negative afterimages of target stimuli. Hofstoetter et al. (2004) found that afterimages were not attenuated by MIB, thus indicating that MIB originates from sites higher than those responsible for visual afterimages. Montaser-Kouhsari et al. (2004) found that information about the orientation of Gabor patches was used even when these patches were invisible during MIB episodes. These authors conclude that MIB originates in an area higher than the primary visual cortex where orientation features are processed.

Therefore, the MIB illusion likely reflects holistic processes which group and organize features of visual input (so-called neurocognitive binding). This view was supported by Hsu et al. (2004), who found that MIB (as well as perceptual filling-in) was enhanced when perceptual grouping between targets and distractors was weak, i.e., when both targets and distractors, taken for themselves, provided clear cues for grouping. Graf et al. (2002) correspondingly showed that MIB can be induced by illusory surfaces originating from Kanizsa elements. This body of results points to a common mechanism underlying reversals in ambiguous figures (cf. Fig. 1) as well as motion-induced blindness.

In the present study, MIB was chosen as a marker of this neurocognitive binding system, because it can be elicited in most participants (including psychiatric patients) and can also be reliably detected. The primary hypothesis specified that MIB is associated with psychopathological symptoms. It was secondarily hypothesized that performance would differ in patients and controls.

Section snippets

Participants

The study sample consisted of 34 patients (27 [79%] men and 7 [21%] women with mean age 27.9 years (S.D. 7.1) and 34 controls (male/female ratio 26:8; mean age 27.9 years, S.D. 8.0; Table 1). Patients were recruited from units of the University Hospital of Social and Community Psychiatry in Bern, Switzerland. 13 inpatients had been admitted to a community-based acute unit (“Soteria Bern”) and 21 patients were undergoing psychiatric outpatient treatment in day hospitals located in Bern. All

Association of MIB with psychopathology

The results of multiple regression analyses in the patient sample are given in Table 2. A significant regression model was derived for MIB rate, with 49% of the total variance ascribable to psychopathology predictors. The association between MIB duration and psychopathology was weaker, however, as only the stepwise regression model was statistically significant. The predictor weights showed that psychopathology factors contributed differentially to MIB perception. The positive and excitement

Discussion

Higher order cognitive processes are based not on external stimuli, but rather on a pre-processed version of these. Pre-attentional stages of cognition are characterized by the self-organization of perceptual input which attaches holistic, gestalt-like attributes to single-stimulus items. The ease and elegance by which such pre-processing occurs is crucial for cognitive performance in subsequent functions, such as the conscious allocation of attention, memory processes, reasoning, or planning.

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by the Swiss National Foundation Grant 32-55954. The authors thank Zeno Kupper for discussions as well as Priscilla Dubouloz and Rahel Meier for their assistance in data acquisition.

References (22)

  • Matussek, P. (1952). Untersuchungen über die Wahrnehmung: 1. Mitteilung. Archive für Psychiatrie und Zeitschrift für...
  • Cited by (39)

    • Examining motion speed processing in schizophrenia using the flash lag illusion

      2020, Schizophrenia Research: Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      Indeed, previous work indicates that perceptual deficits could be present only in patients with pronounced symptomatology (Keane et al., 2013; Silverstein, 2016). It should be noted that we did observe a trend towards a reduced Flash Lag illusion in our patients, which would be indicative of a specific deficit in motion processing in schizophrenia (Tibber et al., 2013; Keane et al., 2013; Silverstein et al., 2013; Tschacher et al., 2006). However, such a dependence on illness state and symptom severity would be at odds with findings that suggests speed discrimination deficits are not directly associated with clinical manifestations in patients, but also present in biological relatives (Chen et al., 1999a).

    • Visceral theory of sleep and origins of mental disorders

      2018, Medical Hypotheses
      Citation Excerpt :

      Sometimes it is considered just as a form of perceptual rivalry because its temporal oscillation pattern of disappearance and reappearance is highly correlated with the pattern reported for binocular rivalry in the same individual [51]. Interestingly but consistently with what was found in binocular rivalry, significantly lower rates of the motion-induced blindness were found in schizophrenia-spectrum disorder participants than in the control group [52]. Somewhat paradoxically, schizophrenia patients show diminished susceptibility to optical illusions.

    • Delusions of control in schizophrenia: Resistant to the mind's best trick?

      2018, Schizophrenia Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Also in other research areas it is frequently observed that schizophrenic patients are less susceptible to many perceptual illusions (Notredame et al., 2014). Schizophrenia is associated with decreased susceptibility to the hollow-mask illusion (Keane et al., 2013; Schneider et al., 2002), the McGurk effect (Pearl et al., 2009; White et al., 2014), contrast illusions (Tadin et al., 2006; Yoon et al., 2009), illusory motion perception (Crawford et al., 2010; Sanders et al., 2013) and motion-induced blindness (Tschacher et al., 2006). All these visual illusions are driven by top-down influences and occur because task-irrelevant contextual information cannot be suppressed.

    • Comprehensive review: Computational modelling of schizophrenia

      2017, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      This spiralling effect would gradually result in stronger, more salient illusions and false-beliefs, eventually leading to full-blown complex hallucinations and deeply anchored delusions. The Bayesian brain hypothesis of schizophrenia has received relatively little empirical testing and validation, with the exception of studies investigating illusions (Crawford et al., 2010; Dima et al., 2009, 2010; Horton and Silverstein, 2011; Keane et al., 2013; Silverstein and Keane, 2011a,b; Tschacher et al., 2006; Williams et al., 2010) or explicit statistical learning (Averbeck et al., 2011; Evans et al., 2012; Freeman et al., 2008, 2014; Garety et al., 2013; Garety and Freeman, 2013; Huq et al., 1988; Joyce et al., 2013; Speechley et al., 2010). Such studies however, tend to investigate either illusions or learning in isolation (i.e. not first the acquisition of expectations through learning and then the influences of these expectations on perception).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text