Healthy adolescent performance on the MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery (MCCB): Developmental data from two samples of volunteers

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2016.02.003Get rights and content

Abstract

The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery (MCCB) fills a significant need for a standardized battery of cognitive tests to use in clinical trials for schizophrenia in adults aged 20–59. A need remains, however, to develop norms for younger individuals, who also show elevated risks for schizophrenia. Toward this end, we assessed performance in healthy adolescents. Baseline MCCB, reading and IQ data were obtained from healthy controls (ages 12–19) participating in two concurrent NIMH-funded studies: North American Prodromal Longitudinal Study phase 2 (NAPLS-2; n = 126) and Boston Center for Intervention Development and Applied Research (CIDAR; n = 13). All MCCB tests were administered except the Managing Emotions subtest from the Mayer–Salovey–Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test. Data were collected from 8 sites across North America. MCCB scores were presented in four 2-year age cohorts as T-scores for each test and cognitive domain, and analyzed for effects of age and sex. Due to IQ differences between age-grouped subsamples, IQ served as a covariate in analyses. Overall and sex-based raw scores for individual MCCB tests are presented for each age-based cohort. Adolescents generally showed improvement with age in most MCCB cognitive domains, with the clearest linear trends in Attention/Vigilance and Working Memory. These control data show that healthy adolescence is a dynamic period for cognitive development that is marked by substantial improvement in MCCB performance through the 12–19 age range. They also provide healthy comparison raw scores to facilitate clinical evaluations of adolescents, including those at risk for developing psychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia-related conditions.

Introduction

Neurocognitive impairments are a central problem for individuals with schizophrenia, beginning in childhood during the premorbid period and continuing throughout life. The development of a pathway for regulatory approval for new treatment strategies to reduce cognitive deficits in schizophrenia created the need for a standardized battery of neuropsychological tests to assess the effectiveness of proposed treatments. A National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) initiative to encourage the development of novel interventions to attenuate cognitive deficits in schizophrenia, called Measurement and Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia (MATRICS), addressed this need. This led to the development of a reliable, valid “MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery” (MCCB) for use in clinical trials and for other purposes (Kern et al., 2008, Nuechterlein et al., 2008). The MCCB is comprised of 9 tests that reflect six distinct, replicated dimensions of neurocognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia, including: Speed of Processing, Attention/Vigilance, Working Memory, Verbal Learning, Visual Learning, and Reasoning and Problem Solving (Nuechterlein et al., 2004). A 10th test, the Managing Emotions Branch of the Mayer–Salovey–Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT), and the additional dimension, Social Cognition, was added because of growing interest in this component of function, and because of its relevance to real world function (Nuechterlein and Green, 2006).

Thus far, investigations into the psychometric properties of the MCCB have been positive. The MCCB demonstrates excellent reliability, minimal practice effects, a low rate of missing data and significant correlations with functional capacity measures (Green et al., 2011, Nuechterlein et al., 2008). Subsequent large, industry-sponsored, multi-site clinical trial studies have affirmed these positive psychometric characteristics (Buchanan et al., 2011, Keefe et al., 2011). The MCCB has also shown strong sensitivity to change in adults with schizophrenia who participated in an intensive cognitive remediation program (Fisher et al., 2009).

Validation studies involving the MCCB have also been encouraging. Progress has been made in demonstrating that improvement on neuropsychological tests such as those in the MCCB is related to improvement in ‘real-world’ tasks (Buchanan et al., 2005, Buchanan et al., 2011). Methods for evaluating performance-based and interview-based measures were developed as part of the MATRICS Psychometric and Standardization Study (PASS) (Green et al., 2008), and supported by the Validation of Intermediate Measures study (VIM) (Green et al., 2011). The success of the MCCB has also been facilitated by its translation into many languages for use in international trials. Indeed, the MATRICS website (http://www.matricsinc.org) listed 21 languages by 2015.

The initial success of the MCCB reliability and validity studies has spurred efforts to characterize the battery further and expand its utility. Recent investigations focused on efforts to refine its factor structure (Burton et al., 2013, McCleery et al., 2015), assess its performance in other psychiatric conditions such as bipolar disorder (Burdick et al., 2011, Sperry et al., 2015), and evaluate its performance in older individuals (Rajji et al., 2013). One area in particular need of further study is the performance of psychiatrically healthy adolescents on the MCCB. While the MCCB norms are stratified for age and sex, and cover the adult age ranges of 20–59 in five year intervals, a substantial proportion of people with schizophrenia experience the onset of their illness (Cannon et al., 1999, Juuhl-Langseth et al., 2014, Thomsen, 1996) or associated, prodromal symptoms of their illness before age 20. The interpretation of the MCCB scores is thus limited in pharmacological and/or psychosocial clinical trials involving youth. Individuals with early-onset schizophrenia (EOS; onset before age 18), including childhood onset (COS; onset by age 13) and adolescent-onset schizophrenia (AOS; onset between ages 13–18), constitute an important clinical population, particularly for early intervention.

There are additional reasons to study cognitive functions in this younger age group. First, the identification of cognitive problems is significant in its own right, regardless of etiology. Second, it is often prognostic, especially in clinical high risk (CHR) adolescent samples (Fusar-Poli et al., 2012a, Fusar-Poli et al., 2012b, Giuliano et al., 2012, Seidman et al., 2010, Woodberry et al., 2008). Moreover, cognitive weaknesses often predict current function in high risk youth (Lin et al., 2011), rates of premorbid abnormalities, familial schizophrenia-spectrum disorders (Nicolson et al., 2003), subsequent exacerbations of cognitive problems (Rajji et al., 2009), greater social disability (Eggers and Bunk, 1997) and functional outcomes over time (Amminger et al., 2011, Ballageer et al., 2005, Hollis, 2000, Insel, 2007, Insel, 2010).

It is thus important to clearly interpret MCCB performance in healthy individuals younger than 20, and particularly in adolescents. Among the few published studies using the MCCB in patients with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders and healthy control subjects below the age of 18, adolescents aged 11–13 years old who showed symptoms of psychosis performed more poorly on several MCCB tasks than adolescents who did not show psychotic symptoms (Kelleher et al., 2013). Similarly, adolescents 12 to 18 years old with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders (mean = 15.8 years) showed stable performance deficits compared to age-matched healthy controls on all MCCB domains except social cognition (Holmen et al., 2010, Juuhl-Langseth et al., 2014).

Still needed, however, are studies of MCCB performance across normal development, both to identify developmental trajectories, and to establish useful comparison data for researchers and clinicians. A Norwegian standardization project reported progress toward this goal by measuring MCCB performance for 5 groups ranging from 12 to 59 years of age, including a 12 to 19 year-old group (n = 50) (Mohn et al., 2012). Similarly, a recent study assessed MCCB performance in 5 groups ranging from 8 to 23 years of age and narrowed the age bands to 4-year bins (e.g. 8 to 11 years; n's = 21 to 54 for the 3 adolescent bins) (Nitzburg et al., 2014). This study provided raw scores and T-scores, the latter of which were derived across all age groups. While both of these studies assessed adolescents, they covered this dynamically complex and heterogeneous stage of development as either a single eight-year period (Mohn et al., 2012), or as 3 four-year periods that also included younger children in the earlier period and adults older than 19 in the later period (Nitzburg et al., 2014). The current study extends the analysis of normal MCCB performance by focusing solely on a teenage sample to assess age-related differences within adolescence, with data presented in 4 two-year age periods that range from ages 12 to 19, further refining healthy comparison data for this period.

We present adolescent control data and examine the influence of age and sex on test performance, comparing our findings with those described in the original adult (aged 20–59) version. Our goal is to expand the utility of the MCCB as a standard battery for adolescents by providing both developmental trajectories for each of its cognitive domains (except social cognition), and control comparison data as raw scores to facilitate clinical evaluations of adolescents who may be at risk for developing psychiatric disorders.

Section snippets

Participants

Participants included 139 adolescents and young adults between the ages of 12–19 who were recruited as healthy control (HC) volunteers from two concurrent NIMH-funded studies: the second phase of the multi-site North American Prodromal Longitudinal Study (NAPLS-2; n = 126; 91% of the overall sample, which included eight sites from four geographic regions), and the single-site Boston Center for Intervention Development and Applied Research (CIDAR) study, “Vulnerability to Progression in

Results

Several distributions of the nine MCCB tasks were skewed and thus transformed as described above. Logarithmically transformed variables included Trail Making Test Part A (following which the direction of the log scores was reversed before T-score conversion), HVLT-R, BVMT-R, and NAB Mazes. Sociodemographic and other characteristics of the overall sample and age cohorts are provided in Table 3. The age range from 12 to 19 provides coverage of the adolescent period of development up to the lower

Discussion

These findings show that performance in MCCB cognitive domains follows a dynamic trajectory of improvement in adolescent NAPLS and CIDAR control subjects that is at least partially independent of overall cognitive ability (i.e. IQ). They also show means and standard deviations for individual test scores throughout adolescence. These data add to a small literature describing the performance of healthy individuals below the age of 20 on the MCCB. It is the first study to focus on adolescents

Role of funding source

This project was a cooperative agreement between the investigator sites and the National Institutes of Health.

Funding

National Institute of Mental Health grants (U01MH0818902 to T.D.C., U01MH081984 to J.M.A., P50MH066286 to C.E.B., U01MH082022 to K.S.C., U01MH081857 to B.A.C., U01MH082004 to D.O.P., U01MH081928 to L.J.S., U01MH081988 to E.F.W., U01MH066160 to S.W.W., P50MH080272 to R.W. M.) and the Commonwealth Research Center of the Massachusetts Department of Mental Health (SCDMH82101008006 to

Contributors

All data was collected by the Principal Investigators of the NAPLS Study (JA, CEB, KSC, TDH, BAC, DHM, THM, DOP, MTT, EFW, SWW, LJS) and the Boston Cidar Study (RWM). WSS, RMG, AJG, and LJS conceptualized the paper and analyses. WSS, RMG, and AJG wrote the first draft of the paper. RMG, AJG, and KAW performed the statistical analyses. WSS, RMG, AJG, KAW, and LJS provided the clinical and neuropsychological training and supervision of staff. LJS provided scientific oversight and reviewed all

Conflict of interest

Dr. Green has been a consultant to AbbVie, Biogen, DSP, EnVivo/Forum and Roche, and he is on the scientific advisory board of Mnemosyne. Dr. Nuechterlein has received unrelated research support from Janssen Scientific Affairs, Genentech, and Brain Plasticity, Inc., and has consulted to Genentech, Otsuka, Janssen, and Brain Plasticity, Inc. All other authors declare that they have no actual or potential conflict of interest including any financial, personal or other relationships with other

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge and thank the study participants and their family members. We also thank the clinical, research assistant, and data management staff from the Boston CIDAR and NAPLS-2 studies, including Caitlin Bryant, Ann Cousins, Grace Francis, Molly Franz, Michelle Friedman-Yakoobian, Lauren Gibson, Andréa Gnong-Granato, Maria Hiraldo, Sarah Hornbach, Matcheri Keshavan, Kristy Klein, Grace Min, Elena Molokotos, Keira O′Donovan, Corin Pilo, Janine Rodenhiser-Hill, Julia Schutt, Rachael Serur,

References (60)

  • T.K. Rajji et al.

    Cognitive performance of individuals with schizophrenia across seven decades: a study using the MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery

    Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatr.

    (2013)
  • R.H.B. Benedict

    Brief Visuospatial Memory Test - Revised

    (1997)
  • J.R. Blair et al.

    Predicting premorbid IQ: A revision of the National Adult Reading Test

    Clinical Neuropsychology

    (1989)
  • J. Brandt et al.

    The Hopkins Verbal Learning Test - Revised

    (2001)
  • R.W. Buchanan et al.

    A summary of the FDA-NIMH-MATRICS workshop on clinical trial design for neurocognitive drugs for schizophrenia

    Schizophr. Bull.

    (2005)
  • R.W. Buchanan et al.

    The FDA-NIMH-MATRICS guidelines for clinical trial design of cognitive-enhancing drugs: what do we know 5 years later?

    Schizophr. Bull.

    (2011)
  • K.E. Burdick et al.

    The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery in patients with bipolar I disorder

    Neuropsychopharmacology

    (2011)
  • M. Cannon et al.

    School performance in Finnish children and later development of schizophrenia: a population-based longitudinal study

    Arch. Gen. Psychiatry

    (1999)
  • C. Eggers et al.

    The long-term course of childhood-onset schizophrenia: a 42-year follow-up

    Schizophr. Bull.

    (1997)
  • M. Fisher et al.

    Using neuroplasticity-based auditory training to improve verbal memory in schizophrenia

    Am. J. Psychiatry

    (2009)
  • M. First et al.

    Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV-TR axis I disorders, research version, non-patient edition (SCID-I/NP)

    (2002)
  • P. Fusar-Poli et al.

    The psychosis high risk state: a comprehensive state-of-the-art review

    JAMA Psychiatry

    (2012)
  • P. Fusar-Poli et al.

    Cognitive functioning in prodromal psychosis: a meta-analysis

    Arch. Gen. Psychiatry

    (2012)
  • A.J. Giuliano et al.

    Neurocognition in psychosis risk syndrome: a quantitative and qualitative review

    Curr. Pharm. Des.

    (2012)
  • J.M. Gold et al.

    Auditory working memory and Wisconsin card sorting test performance in schizophrenia

    Arch. Gen. Psychiatry

    (1997)
  • M.F. Green et al.

    Functional co-primary measures for clinical trials in schizophrenia: results from the MATRICS psychometric and standardization study

    Am. J. Psychiatry

    (2008)
  • M.F. Green et al.

    Evaluation of co-primary measures for clinical trials of cognition enhancement in schizophrenia

    Am. J. Psychiatry

    (2011)
  • D.J. Heyanka et al.

    Intraindividual neuropsychological test variability in healthy individuals with high average intelligence and educational attainment

    Int. J. Neurosci.

    (2013)
  • D. Hien et al.

    Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV-child edition (version 1.0)

    (1994)
  • C. Hollis

    Adolescent schizophrenia

    Adv. Psychiatr. Treat.

    (2000)
  • Cited by (21)

    • Cognitive functioning in early-onset psychosis

      2023, Adolescent Psychosis: Clinical and Scientific Perspectives
    • Prevalence, profile and associations of cognitive impairment in Ugandan first-episode psychosis patients

      2022, Schizophrenia Research: Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      There are also differing clinical, environmental, and sociodemographic exposures in low resource settings, yet how they are associated with cognitive impairment is still limited. For example, previous work and education history may be the best source of a patient's previous level of cognitive functioning when determining if there is a current decline, yet their association with cognitive impairment has not been well described in low resource settings (Stone et al., 2020; Stone et al., 2016). The association between cognitive impairment and clinical characteristics that are more prevalent in FEP patients from low resource settings like longer duration of untreated psychosis (DUP), higher rates of childhood trauma, and greater psychosis severity are unclear (Aas et al., 2013; Kilian et al., 2018; Lezak et al., 2004; Fawzi et al., 2013; Hecker et al., 2015).

    • Neurodegenerative model of schizophrenia: Growing evidence to support a revisit

      2022, Schizophrenia Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Cognitive abilities change throughout life. They strengthen during development (Stone et al., 2016; Waber et al., 2007), plateau in early adulthood and then different cognitive functions (domains) start declining at different ages and rates (Salthouse, 2019). Notably, normal age-related performance decrements in multiple cognitive abilities (e.g., reasoning, memory, and processing speed) are first evident in the 3rd and 4th decades of life (Salthouse, 2009) — not just in middle or old age.

    • Neurocognition as a Biomarker for Psychosis Onset: Exploring the Impact of Age

      2022, Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging
    • Neurocognitive Assessments Are More Important Among Adolescents Than Adults for Predicting Psychosis in Clinical High Risk

      2022, Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging
      Citation Excerpt :

      A possible explanation for such diverse neurocognitive performances between adolescent and adult HC subjects were not replicated in CHR participants, which may be due to floor effects caused by the broad and significant neurocognitive deficits in the CHR sample. Consistent with findings from an adolescent HC sample (ages 12–19 years) of NAPLS-2, adolescents generally showed improvement with age in most MCCB cognitive domains, except BVMT-R and HVLT-R (39). The BVMT-R score did not change significantly in adolescent HC subjects of NAPLS-2, contrary to our adolescent HC subjects who performed significantly better than the adult group in the BVMT-R test.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text