Elsevier

Schizophrenia Research

Volume 86, Issues 1–3, September 2006, Pages 181-188
Schizophrenia Research

Emotion and psychosis: Links between depression, self-esteem, negative schematic beliefs and delusions and hallucinations

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2006.06.018Get rights and content

Abstract

Background

The role of emotion in psychosis is being increasingly recognised. Cognitive conceptualisations of psychosis (e.g. [Garety, P.A., Kuipers, E.K., Fowler, D., Freeman, D., Bebbington, P.E., 2001. A cognitive model of the positive symptoms of psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 31, 189–195]) emphasise a central, normal, direct and non-defensive role for negative emotion in the development and maintenance of psychosis. This study tests specific predictions made by Garety et al. [Garety, P.A., Kuipers, E.K., Fowler, D., Freeman, D., Bebbington, P.E., 2001. A cognitive model of the positive symptoms of psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 31, 189–195] about the role of emotion and negative evaluative beliefs in psychosis.

Methods

100 participants who had suffered a recent relapse in psychosis were recruited at baseline for the Prevention of Relapse in Psychosis (PRP) trial. In a cross-sectional analysis, we examined the role of depression, self-esteem and negative evaluative beliefs in relation to specific positive symptoms (persecutory delusions, auditory hallucinations and grandiose delusions) and symptom dimensions (e.g. distress, negative content, pre-occupation and conviction).

Results

Analysis indicated that individuals with more depression and lower self-esteem had auditory hallucinations of greater severity and more intensely negative content, and were more distressed by them. In addition, individuals with more depression, lower self-esteem and more negative evaluations about themselves and others had persecutory delusions of greater severity and were more pre-occupied and distressed by them. The severity of grandiose delusions was related inversely to depression scores and negative evaluations about self, and directly to higher self-esteem.

Conclusions

This study provides evidence for the role of emotion in schizophrenia spectrum-disorders. Mood, self-esteem and negative evaluative beliefs should be considered when conceptualising psychosis and designing interventions.

Introduction

The role of emotion in the development and maintenance of psychosis is being increasingly recognised (e.g. Birchwood, 2003, Birchwood and Trower, 2006, Freeman and Garety, 2003, Guillem et al., 2005, Hafner et al., 2005). There is now a body of evidence from epidemiological, questionnaire, experimental and treatment studies that low mood, low self-esteem and negative schematic beliefs can contribute to the development of symptoms of psychosis (e.g. Barrowclough et al., 2003, Bowins and Shugar, 1998, Close and Garety, 1998, Drake et al., 2004, Freeman et al., 1998, Freeman et al., 2003, Guillem et al., 2005, Hafner et al., 2005, Hall and Tarrier, 2003, Iqbal et al., 2000, Krabbendam et al., 2002, Krabbendam et al., 2005, Martin and Penn, 2001, Trower and Chadwick, 1995). Krabbendam et al. (2005) reported a study of over 4500 individuals screened for psychiatric status and followed up for 3 years. Given the presence of hallucinatory experiences at baseline, the increase in risk of psychosis outcome at Year 3 was higher in those with depressed mood at Year 1 than in those without depressed mood at Year 1. Barrowclough et al. (2003) assessed negative self-evaluation using an in-depth interview in a group with schizophrenia (N = 59). They found that negative self-evaluation was strongly associated with the positive symptoms of psychosis (PANSS positive sub-scale). Importantly, this remained significant even when levels of depression were controlled.

However, recent cognitive conceptualisations of psychosis (e.g. Bentall et al., 1994, Chadwick and Birchwood, 1994, Garety et al., 2001, Trower and Chadwick, 1995) vary in their account of the role of emotion in psychosis. Bentall and colleagues understand persecutory delusions to be the result of a psychological defence against underlying negative emotion and low self-esteem (e.g. Bentall et al., 1994). In contrast, Garety and colleagues claim negative emotion and low self-esteem have a central, normal, direct and non-defensive role in the development of symptoms (Fowler, 2000, Freeman and Garety, 2003, Garety et al., 2001). They hypothesise that as emotional disorder increases, psychotic symptoms worsen and that this is ‘normal’ (i.e. the same emotional processes are likely to be operating with the same direction of effect as in the non-psychotic population).

Building on the work of other researchers (e.g. Maher, 1988, Frith, 1992, Hemsleyr, 1993, Bentall et al., 1994, Chadwick and Birchwood, 1994, Morrison et al., 1995), Garety et al. (2001) proposed that emotional changes occur in the context of anomalous conscious experiences (e.g. heightened perceptions, thoughts experienced as voices) and adverse life events. Such emotional changes feed back into the moment-by-moment processing of anomalous experiences, influence their content, and perpetuate their occurrence. Fowler (2000) specifically suggests that distressing voices and persecutory delusions are associated with the appraisal of negative beliefs and thinking. For example, the content of distressing auditory hallucinations often mirrors the content of depressive thinking associated with low mood (Fowler, 2000).

Garety et al. (2001) also propose that these emotional processes occur against an important social and cognitive background. Early adverse experiences are postulated to create an enduring cognitive vulnerability, characterised by negative schematic models of the self and others (e.g. I am vulnerable, others are dangerous). Fowler (2000) suggests that the triggering of negative schematic beliefs in individuals vulnerable to psychosis may lead to them hearing voices with threatening or critical content. Such a view suggests that it is also the accessing of negative schematic beliefs and thoughts of negative content, rather than simply depressed mood or low self-esteem that is associated with distressing voices and persecutory delusions.

Despite these developments, it remains unclear how negative schematic beliefs and emotional dysfunction interact in psychosis. Using a large sample, the current study aims to test predictions made by Garety et al. (2001) about the role of emotion and negative schematic beliefs in psychosis. It builds on previous important work (e.g. Barrowclough et al., 2003) by extending analysis to individual symptoms and to symptom dimensions.

To facilitate this, we developed a new measure of schematic beliefs in psychosis. The Brief Core Schema Scales (BCSS); (Fowler et al., 2006) assess strongly held negative evaluations of self (e.g. I am weak, I am bad, I am useless) and strongly held negative evaluations of others (e.g. Others are untrustworthy, others are dishonest, others are threatening), as well as positive evaluations of self and others. The reason for developing this new measure was two-fold. First, there is a need for clinically relevant, relatively quick self-report measures of schematic beliefs (of self and others) in large studies involving multiple assessments. The BCSS is therefore clinically derived and aims capture beliefs about self and others that are often reported by clients to their therapists. Secondly, we wanted to make clear distinctions between self-report measures of self-esteem (e.g. Rosenberg, 1965) and of schematic beliefs. The negative self-items in the BCSS measure strongly held negative self-evaluations and provide an operational construct of negative schematic self-beliefs. These are distinct from existing assessments of self-esteem that tend to measure presence of positive evaluations of self or their absence, and seem to relate closely to depressed mood. As the current study aims to test predictions about the role of negative schematic beliefs in psychosis, the positive evaluative belief scales of the BCSS are not investigated here.

Hence, in this study (in accordance with Garety et al., 2001) we predicted normal, direct and non-defensive associations between individual symptoms (and symptom dimensions) and depression, low self-esteem and negative schematic beliefs. We first predicted that the severity and distress/negative content dimensions of auditory hallucinations would be associated with both depression and low self-esteem. This is consistent with suggestions that the content of distressing auditory hallucinations specifically mirror the content of depressive thinking associated with low mood (Fowler, 2000). The direction of the relationship between auditory hallucinations and low mood/low self-esteem was predicted to be driven both by the auditory hallucinations and the resultant negative affect; a vicious cycle.

Our second prediction was that while there would be associations between low mood, low self-esteem, and the severity and distress dimensions of persecutory delusions, negative evaluative beliefs about self and others would remain independently associated with persecutory delusions once the confounding effects of depression and self-esteem were controlled. This would support theoretical proposals (e.g. Fowler, 2000, Garety et al., 2001, Birchwood, 2003) and empirical evidence (e.g. Barrowclough et al., 2003) indicating a role for schematic beliefs in psychotic symptoms independent of mood.

Garety et al. (2001) do not make specific predictions about the role of emotion in grandiose beliefs, although Freeman and Garety (2003) hypothesise that expansive or elated mood could build upon pre-existing inflated (or accurate) perceptions of self. This positive mood state might then reinforce and amplify aspects of the self-concept resulting in a grandiose belief. Smith et al. (2005) present data that support this view. Our third prediction was therefore that grandiose delusions would be associated with the absence of depressed mood and with elevated or positive views of self (self-esteem), a pattern of results we have found in our non-clinical study (Fowler et al., 2006). More speculatively, we also hypothesised that grandiose delusions in a clinical sample would be combined with negative evaluative beliefs about others. Our reasoning for this was that a combination of elevated mood and positive views of self with negative evaluations of others may foster a social position that maintains positive self-evaluations, whilst ignoring social cues, thus leading to the maintenance of grandiose delusions.

Section snippets

Methods

The Psychological Prevention of Relapse in Psychosis (PRP) Trial is a British multi-centre randomised controlled trial of cognitive behavioural therapy and family intervention for psychosis (ISRCTN83557988). The detailed methods of the PRP trial are published elsewhere (Freeman et al., 2004). For the present study, participants were assessed before randomisation to trial conditions took place.

Demographic and clinical data

Sixty-eight percent of the sample was male and the mean age was 39 years (S.D. = 10.9 years), range = 19–65. Sixty-nine percent had been admitted to hospital as a result of their recent relapse in psychosis. Almost 70% described themselves as White-British, 10% as Black-Caribbean, 7% as Black-African and 11% as from other ethic backgrounds. Seventy eight percent had a diagnosis of schizophrenia, 20% of schizoaffective disorder and 2% of delusional disorder. The mean length of illness was 11.7 years

Discussion

The current findings are consistent with some existing empirical evidence (e.g. Krabbendam et al., 2005) that low mood, low self-esteem and negative schematic beliefs can contribute to the development of symptoms of psychosis. Some of our predictions of normal, direct and non-defensive associations between individual symptoms (and symptom dimensions) and depression, low self-esteem and negative schematic beliefs were supported.

We first predicted that the severity and distress/negative content

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all the participants who took part in the study from both London and East Anglia and all the staff who helped in recruitment.

This study was supported by a Wellcome Trust Programme Grant: 062452. PRP is registered as ISRCTN: 83557988.

References (39)

  • M. Birchwood

    Pathways to emotional dysfunction in first-episode psychosis

    British Journal of Psychiatry

    (2003)
  • M. Birchwood et al.

    The future of cognitive–behavioural therapy for psychosis: not a quasi-neuroleptic

    British Journal of Psychiatry

    (2006)
  • B. Bowins et al.

    Delusions and self-esteem

    Canadian Journal of Psychiatry

    (1998)
  • P.D.J. Chadwick et al.

    The omnipotence of voices: a cognitive approach to hallucinations

    British Journal of Psychiatry

    (1994)
  • H. Close et al.

    Cognitive assessment of voices: further developments in understanding the emotional impact of voices

    British Journal of Clinical Psychology

    (1998)
  • R.J Drake et al.

    The evolution of insight, paranoia and depression during early schizophrenia

    Psychological Medicine

    (2004)
  • D. Fowler

    Cognitive behaviour therapy for psychosis: from understanding to treatment

    Psychiatric Rehabilitation Skills

    (2000)
  • D.G. Fowler et al.

    The Brief Core Schema Scales (BCSS): psychometric properties and associations with paranoia and grandiosity in non-clinical and psychosis samples

    Psychological Medicine

    (2006)
  • D. Freeman et al.

    The London–East Anglia randomised controlled trial of cognitive–behaviour therapy for psychosis: IV. Self-esteem and persecutory delusions

    British Journal of Clinical Psychology

    (1998)
  • Cited by (329)

    • Synthetic surprise as the foundation of the psychedelic experience

      2024, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    Tel.: +44 1603 593 601; fax: +44 1603 593 604.

    View full text